Wednesday, February 11, 2015
Place Logic
Monday, September 24, 2012
At Large in the Library: Geosophy
Monday, June 7, 2010
Outrage Revisited
The Guardian's current architecture critic Jonathan Glancey has been creating a series of films (begun 19 May 2010) that follow Nairn's footsteps across the British countryside. Watch the installments here.
Tuesday, February 2, 2010
Geography & Building Types
Tuesday, December 22, 2009
State of Happiness
Posted by the University of Warwick (United Kingdom):
"New research by the UK’s University of Warwick and Hamilton College in the US has used the happiness levels of a million individual US citizens to discover which are the best and worst states in which to live in the United States. New York and Connecticut come bottom of a life-satisfaction league table, and Hawaii and Louisiana are at the top. The analysis reveals also that happiness levels closely correlate with objective factors such as congestion and air quality across the US’s 50 states.
The new research published in the elite journal Science on 17th December 2009 is by Professor Andrew Oswald of the UK’s University of Warwick and Stephen Wu of Hamilton College in the US. It provides the first external validation of people’s self-reported levels of happiness. “We would like to think this is a breakthrough. It provides an justification for the use of subjective well-being surveys in the design of government policies, and will be of value to future economic and clinical researchers across a variety of fields in science and social science” said Professor Oswald.
The researchers examined a 2005- 2008 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System random sample of 1.3 million United States citizens in which life-satisfaction in each U.S. state was measured. This provided a league table of happiness by US State reproduced below. The researchers decided to use the data to try to resolve one of the most significant issues facing economists and clinical scientists carrying out research into human well-being.
Researchers have to rely on people’s self declared levels of happiness – but how can one trust those self declarations? There have been studies that try to match declared levels of happiness to clinical signs of stress such as blood pressure. That has been useful, but one cannot know for sure whether those physiological signs are driving happiness or whether the reverse is true. Researchers have, for decades, longed for a more clearly external scientific check on, and corroboration of, well-being survey answers.
The two researchers stumbled on a parallel approach that allowed them to do such a check. They discovered research by Stuart Gabriel and colleagues from UCLA published in 2003 which considered objective indicators for each individual State of the USA such as: precipitation; temperature; wind speed; sunshine; coastal land; inland water; public land; National Parks; hazardous waste sites; environmental ‘greenness’; commuting time; violent crime; air quality; student-teacher ratio; local taxes; local spending on education and highways; cost of living. This allowed the creation of a rank order of US states showing which should provide the happiest living experience. This was a truly external data source that could be used to check the self declared levels of happiness; Gabriel’s team had no happiness data in 2003 that could allow the check to be completed.
But Professors Oswald and Wu were able to do the first state-by-state USA happiness calculations. They then obtained Gabriel’s numbers. When the two rankings were compared, they found a close correlation between people’s subjective life-satisfaction scores and objectively estimated quality of life.
The lead author on the study, Professor Andrew Oswald from the University of Warwick, said:
'The beauty of this statistical method is that we are able to look below the surface of American life -- to identify the deep patterns in people's underlying life satisfaction and happiness from Alabama to Wyoming. The type of study is new to the United States. We are the first to be able to do this calculation -- partly because we are fortunate enough to have a random anonymized sample of 1.3 million Americans. But we could not have done it without the early painstaking work by Gabriel’s team.'
'The state-by-state pattern is of interest in itself. But it also matters scientifically. We wanted to study whether people's feelings of satisfaction with their own lives are reliable, that is, whether they match up to reality -- of sunshine hours, congestion, air quality, etc -- in their own state. And they do match. When human beings give you an answer on a numerical scale about how satisfied they are with their lives, you should pay attention.
People’s happiness answers are true, you might say. This suggests that life-satisfaction survey data might be tremendously useful for governments to use in the design of economic and social policies.' said Oswald.
Professor Oswald expressed caution in how some of the exact results should be interpreted – for example, for the state of Louisiana in the survey following the disruption in caused by Hurricane Katrina, but was confident that the data on most states was a true reflection of well-being levels saying:
'We have been asked a lot whether we expected that states like New York and California would do so badly in the happiness ranking. Having visited and lived in various parts of the US, I am only a little surprised. Many people think these states would be marvellous places to live in. The problem is that if too many individuals think that way, they move into those states, and the resulting congestion and house prices make it a non-fulfilling prophecy. In a way, it is like the stock market. If everyone thinks it would be great to buy stock X, that stock is generally already overvalued. Bargains in life are usually found outside the spotlight. It seems that exactly the same is true of the best places to live.'"
1 | Louisiana |
2 | Hawaii |
3 | Florida |
4 | Tennessee |
5 | Arizona |
6 | Mississippi |
7 | Montana |
8 | South Carolina |
9 | Alabama |
10 | Maine |
11 | Alaska |
12 | North Carolina |
13 | Wyoming |
14 | Idaho |
15 | South Dakota |
16 | Texas |
17 | Arkansas |
18 | Vermont |
19 | Georgia |
20 | Oklahoma |
21 | Colorado |
22 | Delaware |
23 | Utah |
24 | New Mexico |
25 | North Dakota |
26 | Minnesota |
27 | New Hampshire |
28 | Virginia |
29 | Wisconsin |
30 | Oregon |
31 | Iowa |
32 | Kansas |
33 | Nebraska |
34 | West Virginia |
35 | Kentucky |
36 | Washington |
37 | District of Columbia |
38 | Missouri |
39 | Nevada |
40 | Maryland |
41 | Pennsylvania |
42 | Rhode Island |
43 | Massachusetts |
44 | Ohio |
45 | Illinois |
46 | California |
47 | Indiana |
48 | Michigan |
49 | New Jersey |
50 | Connecticut |
51 | New York |
Thursday, March 26, 2009
Red River Rising
Monday, February 16, 2009
Opening Soon: In-Situ
Thursday, January 22, 2009
CLUI & MJT
Monday, December 22, 2008
The Blizzard of 1966
Monday, December 8, 2008
Bibliophilia-topophilia
Entry postmark deadline: January 31, 2009
Juror: Natasha Lovelace, Kennesaw State University
Entry Fee: $25 for 3 entries
For details, prospectus and entry form, please email:
Charlotte@OTACenter.com
Charlotte Lowry Collins, Director
Olde Towne Arts Center
300 Robert St., Slidell, LA 70458
Charlotte@OTACenter.com
985-649-0555
Thursday, December 4, 2008
Experimental Geography
Geography benefits from the study of specific histories, sites, and memories. Every estuary, land fill, and cul-de-sac has a story to tell. The task of the geographer is to alert us to what is directly in front of us, while the task of the experimental geographer—an amalgam of scientist, artist, and explorer—is to do so in a manner that deploys aesthetics, ambiguity, poetry, and a dash of empiricism. This exhibition explores the distinctions between geographical study and artistic experience of the earth, as well as the juncture where the two realms collide (and possibly make a new field altogether).
http://www.ici-exhibitions.
To order the exhibition catalog:
http://mhpbooks.com/book.php?id=166
The exhibition's itinerary:
Rochester Art Center, Rochester, Minnesota
February 7 April 18, 2009
The Albuquerque Museum, Albuquerque, New Mexico
June 28 September 20, 2009
AVAILABLE
October 2009 - January 2010
Colby College Museum of Art, Waterville, Maine
February 21 - May 30, 2010
AVAILABLE
June - August 2010
Image above: The Center for Land Use Interpretation, Untitled (image and text panels depicting the programs and projects of CLUI), 2007.